adventr: repeated-measures designs

library(forcats)
library(learnr)
library(tidyverse)
library(BayesFactor)
library(nlme)
library(WRS2)

knitr::opts_chunk$set(echo = FALSE, warning = FALSE)
tutorial_options(exercise.cap = "Exercise")

hint_text <- function(text, text_color = "#E69F00"){
  hint <- paste("<font color='", text_color, "'>", text, "</font>", sep = "")
  return(hint)
}

#Read dat files needed for the tutorial

calcite_tib <- adventr::calcite_dat

#setup objects for code blocks

calcite_tidy <- calcite_tib %>% gather(key = "face_type", value = "recognition", -id)

An Adventure in R: Repeated-measures designs (part 1)

Overview

This tutorial is one of a series that accompanies An Adventure in Statistics [@RN10163] by me, Andy Field. These tutorials contain abridged sections from the book so there are some copyright considerations but I offer them under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, ^[Basically you can use this tutorial for teaching and non-profit activities but do not meddle with it or claim it as your own work.]

Story précis

Why a précis?

Because these tutorials accompany my book An adventure in statistics, which uses a fictional narrative to teach the statistics, some of the examples might not make sense unless you know something about the story. For those of you who don't have the book I begin each tutorial with a précis of the story. If you're not interested then fair enough - click past this section.

General context for the story

It is the future. Zach, a rock musician and Alice, a geneticist, who have been together since high school live together in Elpis, the ‘City of Hope’.

Zach and Alice were born in the wake of the Reality Revolution which occurred after a Professor Milton Gray invented the Reality Prism – a transparent pyramid worn on the head – that brought honesty to the world. Propaganda and media spin became unsustainable, religions collapsed, advertising failed. Society could no longer be lied to. Everyone could know the truth about anything that they could look at. A gift, some said, to a previously self-interested, self-obsessed society in which the collective good had been eroded.

But also a curse. For, it soon became apparent that through this Reality Prism, people could no longer kid themselves about their own puffed-up selves as they could see what they were really like – by and large, pretty ordinary. And this caused mass depression. People lost faith in themselves. Artists abandoned their pursuits, believing they were untalented and worthless.

Zach and Alice have never worn a Reality Prism and have no concept of their limitations. They were born after the World Governance Agency (WGA) destroyed all Reality Prisms, along with many other pre-revolution technologies, with the aim of restoring community and well-being. However, this has not been straightforward and in this post-Prism world, society has split into pretty much two factions

Everyone has a star, a limitless space on which to store their digital world.

Zach and Alice are Clocktarians. Their technology consists mainly of:

Main Protagonists

How Zach's adventure begins

Alice has been acting strangely, on edge for weeks, disconnected and uncommunicative, as if she is hiding something and Zach can’t get through to her. Arriving home from band practice, unusually, she already home and listening to an old album that the two of them enjoyed together, back in a simpler, less complicated time in their relationship. During an increasingly testy evening, that involves a discussion with the Head about whether or not a Proteus causes brain cancer, Alice is interrupted by an urgent call which she takes in private. She returns looking worried and is once again, distracted. She tells Zach that she has ‘a big decision to make’. Before going to bed, Zach asks her if he can help with the decision but she says he ‘already has’, thanking him for making ‘everything easier.’ He has no idea what she means and goes to sleep, uneasy.

On waking, Zach senses that something is wrong. And he is right. Alice has disappeared. Her clothes, her possessions and every photo of them together have gone. He can’t get hold of any of her family or friends as their contact information is stored on her Proteus, not on his diePad. He manages to contact the Beimeni Centre but is told that no one by the name of Alice Nightingale has ever worked there. He logs into their constellation but her star has gone. He calls her but finds that her number never existed. She has, thinks Zach, been ‘wiped from the planet.’ He summons The Head but he can’t find her either. He tells Zach that there are three possibilities: Alice has doesn’t want to be found, someone else doesn’t want her to be found or she never existed.

Zach calls his friend Nick, fellow band member and fan of the WGA-installed Repositories, vast underground repositories of actual film, books, art and music. Nick is a Chipper – solely for the purpose of promoting the band using memoryBank – and he puts the word out to their fans about Alice missing.

Thinking as hard as he can, Zach recalls the lyrics of the song she’d been playing the previous evening. Maybe they are significant? It may well be a farewell message and the Head is right. In searching for clues, he comes across a ‘memory stone’ which tells him to read what’s on there. File 1 is a research paper that Zach can’t fathom. It’s written in the ‘language of science’ and the Head offers to help Zach translate it and tells him that it looks like the results of her current work were ‘gonna blow the world’. Zach resolves to do ‘something sensible’ with the report.

Zach doesn’t want to believe that Alice has simply just left him. Rather, that someone has taken her and tried to erase her from the world. He decides to find her therapist, Dr Murali Genari and get Alice’s file. As he breaks into his office, Dr Genari comes up behind him and demands to know what he is doing. He is shaking but not with rage – with fear of Zach. Dr Genari turns out to be friendly and invites Zach to talk to him. Together they explore the possibilities of where Alice might have gone and the likelihood, rating her relationship satisfaction, that she has left him. During their discussion Zach is interrupted by a message on his diePad from someone called Milton. Zach is baffled as to who he is and how he knows that he is currently discussing reverse scoring. Out of the corner of his eye, he spots a ginger cat jumping down from the window ledge outside. The counsellor has to go but suggests that Zach and ‘his new friend Milton’ could try and work things out.

Packages and data

Packages

This tutorial uses the following packages:

These packages are automatically loaded within this tutorial. If you are working outside of this tutorial (i.e. in RStudio) then you need to make sure that the package has been installed by executing install.packages("package_name"), where package_name is the name of the package. If the package is already installed, then you need to reference it in your current session by executing library(package_name), where package_name is the name of the package.

Data

This tutorial has the data files pre-loaded so you shouldn't need to do anything to access the data from within the tutorial. However, if you want to play around with what you have learnt in this tutorial outside of the tutorial environment (i.e. in a stand-alone RStudio session) you will need to download the data files and then read them into your R session. This tutorial uses the following file:

You can load the file in several ways:

Categorical predictors with repeated measures

The model

During Zach's visit to the JIG:SAW complex he visits several research buildings. In the first, he discovers experiments being conducted related to invisibility. The second experiment he learns about involves a repeated-measures design. The researchers asked the question of whether they could manipulate the face perceived by a participant when viewing someone wearing calcite invisibility paste. Participants were all so-called Chippers, who had ID chips installed in their brains. Each participant met 40 people who had calcite paste on their face. For each of these people we took a photo of their face (the ‘actual’ face), and found a photo of a different face matched for sex and age (the ‘different’ face). The researchers could send these images to the participant’s visual cortex using their ID chip. For 20 of the encounters an image of the person’s ‘actual’ face was transmitted to the participant’s ID chip during the encounter, for the remaining 20 encounters the ‘different’ face was sent to the chip. As such the design was repeated measures (each participant experienced both the 'actual' and 'different' conditions). During each encounter the participant was shown the photos of the actual and different face and pointed to the one depicting whom they thought they were meeting. Participants scored a point for each person they correctly identified: a score of 0 would mean that they never chose the correct photo, a score of 20 would mean that they always chose the correct photo. The prediction was that the paste would lead to lower recognition when a different face (inconsistent information) was transmitted to the visual cortex than when the actual face (consistent information) was transmitted. The data are in the tibble calcite_tib, which has three variables:

The data has a hierarchical structure because recognition scores are nested within participants (Figure 1). We, therefore, need to model the individual differences in recognition scores (random intercept) and the effect of the type of face (random slopes).

Figure 1: The structure of the calcite paste data

The model we're fitting is described by the following equation:

$$ \begin{aligned} \text{recognition}{ij} & = b{0i} + b_{1i}\text{face type}{ij}+ ε{ij}\ b_{0i} &= b_0 + u_{0i}\ b_{1i} &= b_1 + u_{1i} \end{aligned} $$

Recognition scores within participants (i) and experimental treatments (j) are predicted from overall recognition scores ($b_0$) and participant's variation around it ($u_0i$), the overall effect of the type of face ($b_1$) and participant's variation around that effect ($u_{1i}$), and some error ($ε_{ij}$). In other words, it is a multilevel model with random intercepts and slopes for the effect of face type.

Making data 'tidy'

Use the code box to inspect the calcite_tib tibble.


calcite_tib   

The data are organised in what is known as the wide format. In this format, each row represents a different entity and columns represent different scores belonging to that entity. For example, row 1 belongs to participant 1, column 2 tells us that they recognized 12 faces when a photo of the actual face was transmitted to their ID chip and column 3 tells us that they recognized 6 faces when a photo of a different face was transmitted to their ID chip. As such, the variable representing the type of face is split across two columns. The wide format is very common, but is messy, and usually we want the data to tidy. Although there is a lot more to it than this, pragmatically, @RN10209 distinguished tidy and messy data structures as follows:

A dataset is messy or tidy depending on how rows, columns and tables are matched up with observations, variables and types. In tidy data:

  1. Each variable forms a column.
  2. Each observation forms a row.
  3. Each type of observational unit forms a table.

... Messy data is any other arrangement of the data.

To make our data 'tidy' we can use the gather() function from the tidyverse suite of packages (specifically tidyr). This function gathers columns together and puts them into a single column (across multiple rows). It takes the form:

gather(data, key = "name_of_column_containing_variable_information", value = "name_of_column_containing_values")

Basically, you enter the name of the tibble that you want to restructure. It will stack all of the values from the columns into a single column with the name that you provide in the value argument, and it will list the original column names in a variable with the name that you provide in the key argument. If you ignore the key and value arguments the function will use the names key and value. To get a feel for what happens, try executing:

tidyr::gather(calcite_tib)

in the code box below.

Our initial tibble had 20 rows (one for each participant) and 3 columns (id, actual, different). You'll see that the restructured tibble has 60 rows (3 for each participant) and 2 columns (one containing the original column names, and one containing the values associated with each participant for the three original columns).

This structure isn't helpful to us because it has gathered all of the original columns, and we need it to gather only the actual and different columns leaving the id column alone (because this column tells us to which participant a score belongs). We can tell the function to leave the id column alone by using a minus sign (-id). We should also define the key and value names. We might call the key variable face_type, because it will represent whether the face beamed to the ID chip was the actual face or a different one, and the value variable recognition because it contains recognition scores (out of 20). Putting all of this together we could execute:

tidyr::gather(calcite_tib, key = "face_type", value = "recognition", -id)

Try this command is the code box. The resulting tibble should have 40 rows (each of 20 participants under each of the two conditions) and three columns (id tells us the participant number, face_type defines the experimental condition, and recognition contains the recognition score for each participant in each experimental condition). These data are tidy. So we can use this restructured tibble we need to save it into an object. Let's call it calcite_tidy. We can do this using a pipe:

calcite_tidy <- calcite_tib %>% 
  tidyr::gather(key = "face_type", value = "recognition", -id)

Execute this command in the code box, and inspect the resulting tibble.


calcite_tidy <- calcite_tib %>%
  tidyr::gather(key = "face_type", value = "recognition", -id)

calcite_tidy

Descriptive data

Use what you've learnt in previous tutorials to create a tibble called calcite_summary containing the mean recognition scores (and their confidence intervals) in the two experimental conditions.

r hint_text("Tip: Remember to use the tidy tibble that we've just created. If you're doing this outside of the tutorial remember to load the packages tidyverse and Hmisc")


calcite_summary <- calcite_tidy %>%
  dplyr::group_by(face_type) %>%
  dplyr::summarize(
    mean_recognition = mean(recognition),
    ci_low_recognition = ggplot2::mean_cl_normal(recognition)$ymin,
    ci_upp_recognition = ggplot2::mean_cl_normal(recognition)$ymax
)
calcite_summary              

Plotting the data

Use the code box below to create an error bar chart with face_type on the x-axis and recognition on the y-axis. If you feel like it, try to superimpose the raw data. (If you do this last part you'll use the geom_point() function, try this, but then add into the function position = position_jitter(width = 0.1, height = 0). This adds what's known as a 'jitter' to the points, which just means that small a random value is added so that the dots don't overlap. By setting width to 0.1, we're limiting the horizontal jitter and disabling the horizontal jitter so that the values on the y-axis are true to the actual scores.) Remember to use the 'tidy' version of the data.


calcite_plot <- ggplot2::ggplot(calcite_tidy, aes(face_type, recognition))
calcite_plot +
  geom_point(colour = "#E69F00", position = position_jitter(width = 0.1, height = 0)) +
  stat_summary(fun.data = "mean_cl_normal", geom = "pointrange") +
  labs(x = "Experimental condition", y = "Recoognition (out of 20") +
  coord_cartesian(ylim = c(0, 20)) +
  scale_y_continuous(breaks = 0:20) +
  scale_x_discrete(labels = c("Actual face", "Different face")) + #adds more descriptive labels to the x-axis
  theme_bw()

Fitting the model

It looks like recognition scores are lower in the different condition. To fit the model we use the lme() function from the nlme package, because we are fitting a linear model with a categorical predictor, but we want to also model the dependency between scores. This function is explained in the tutorial adventr_mlm, just to recap it takes the following general form (I've retained only the key options):

new_object <- lme(outcome ~ predictors, random = formula, data = tibble, method = "REML", na.action = na.fail)

We could, therefore, specify the model as:

calcite_rs <- nlme::lme(recognition ~ face_type, random = ~ face_type|id, data = calcite_tidy, method = "ML")
summary(calcite_rs)
intervals(calcite_rs, which = "fixed")

The first line creates an object called calcite_rs which predicts recognition scores from the effect of face_type but with intercepts and slopes allowed to vary across participants (I appended _rs to the name to remind me it is a random slopes model). The summary() function gives me a summary of the model (including a t-statistic and p-value for the fixed effect of face_type) and intervals() gives me 95% confidence intervals for the fixed effects (hence the which = "fixed"), which in this case are the intercept and effect of face_type.

Try executing these commands in the code box.


calcite_rs <- nlme::lme(recognition ~ face_type, random = ~ face_type|id, data = calcite_tidy, method = "ML")
summary(calcite_rs)
intervals(calcite_rs, which = "fixed")

The output provides estimates of the model parameters (the b-values) and the significance of these values. The Y intercept ($b_0$) is 9.20, which is the value of recognition when face_type = 0. R will have coded the variable face_type using dummy coding. It will do this alphabetically (unless you tell it something different), which means that it will have coded 'actual' as 0 and 'different' as 1 (because a comes before d in the alphabet). Note that 9.20 is the value of the mean in the actual condition that we calculated earlier.

quiz(
  question("How would we interpret the *Value* (-2.35) for *face_typedifferent*? [Select **two** correct answers.]",
    answer("As the value of **face_type** changed from 0 (actual) to 1 (different), recognition scores decrease by 2.35.", correct = T),
    answer("The difference between group means is -2.35.", correct = T),
    answer("As the value of **face_type** changed from 0 (actual) to 1 (different), recognition scores decrease by 2.35 of a standard deviation", message = "This describes the *standardized* B, not the *unstandardized*."),
    answer("Group membership explains -2.35% of the variance in recognition scores", message = sprintf("This is what $R^2$ tells us and you can't have a negative percentage of variance explained!")),
    correct = "Correct - well done!",
    random_answer_order = TRUE,
    allow_retry = T
  )
)

The confidence interval for the effect of face_type tells us that (assuming that this is one of the 95% of samples that yields a confidence interval containing the population value) the true difference between recognition scores when a picture of the actual person's face was used compared to a different face lies between -4.596 and -0.104. This means it could be anything between a very small effect (practically zero) to a fairly large one (4 to 5 fewer faces recognised). The p-value suggests that this difference is significant at p = 0.046. In other words, we can significantly predict recognition scores from the type of face implanted while the participant met people with calcite paste on their face.

A more traditional approach

We have just used a hierarchical linear model to compare two means using a t-statistic. You have likely been taught about t-tests as tests in their own right, and also taught that to compare dependent means you should use the paired (dependent) t-test. You can do the paired t-test in R using the t.test() function described in adventr_15:

new_model <- t.test(outcome ~ predictor, data = tibble, paired = FALSE, var.equal = FALSE, conf.level = 0.95, na.action)

In which:

To get a t-test for the current data we would execute:

t.test(recognition ~ face_type, data = calcite_tidy, paired = TRUE)

or to create an object that we can use later:

calcite_t <- t.test(recognition ~ face_type, data = calcite_tidy, paired = TRUE)
calcite_t

Try this in the code box:


calcite_t <- t.test(recognition ~ face_type, data = calcite_tidy, paired = TRUE)
calcite_t

The value of t (2.1346) and the corresponding p (0.04604) are identical to the linear model that we have already fitted. However, for more complicated designs the linear model has many benefits.

Robust models

The WRS2 package [@RN10205] wraps up a few of the many functions described by Wilcox to perform robust variants of tests [@RN5244]. We'll look at one function that compare two dependent means:

yuend(x, y, tr = 0.2)

The arguments are:

To specify x and y we need to extract them from our tibble. (I will describe one approach that is consistent with using tidy data, but there are others.) We would start by creating separate objects for the scores in the actual and different conditions, using select() and filter(), then insert these into the yuend() function:

actual <- calcite_tidy %>%
  dplyr::filter(face_type == "actual") %>%
  dplyr::select(recognition)

different <- calcite_tidy %>%
  dplyr::filter(face_type == "different") %>%
  dplyr::select(recognition)

WRS2::yuend(different, actual, tr = 0.2)

The first line takes the calcite_tidy tibble and filters it to retain only the scores relating to when the variable face_type has a value equal to "actual", it then selects the variable recognition, which contains the recognition scores. This pipe basically extracts the 20 recognition scores from the actual condition and stores them in a tibble called actual. The second command does the same but extracts the 20 recognition scores from the different condition and stores them in a tibble called different. The third lines applies the yuend() to the two tibbles we have just created. (You can exclude tr = 0.2 because it is the default but I have left it there to make explicit that we are performing a 20% trim.)

Execute these commands in the code box (if stuck, click )


actual <- calcite_tidy %>%
  dplyr::filter(face_type == "actual") %>%
  dplyr::select(recognition)

different <- calcite_tidy %>%
  dplyr::filter(face_type == "different") %>%
  dplyr::select(recognition)

WRS2::yuend(different, actual, tr = 0.2)

Note that the trimmed mean difference is -2.75 (rather than the raw mean difference of -2.35). This difference is not significant because p = 0.087 and the confidence intervals cross zero, indicating that if this is one of the 95% of samples where the confidence interval contains the population value, then the population difference between means could be positive, negative or zero.

The effect size is a robust variant of Cohen's d so can be interpreted as implanting a different face producing recognition scores 0.52 of a standard deviation below that for the actual face.

Bayesian models

Bayes factors

Like in adventr_15 we can use the ttestBF() function from the BayesFactor package. This function was explained in adventr_15, but with repeated measures data we can't use formula input:

object = ttestBF(x, y, data = tibble, paired = FALSE, rscale = "medium")

To recap, by default the function assumes independent scores (paired = FALSE) so for dependent scores we'd change this to paired = TRUE. The function uses default priors that can be specified as a number or as "medium" (the default), "wide", and "ultrawide". These labels correspond to r scale values of $^\sqrt{2}/_2$, 1, and $\sqrt{2}$. These priors were explained in adventr_11.

To specify x and y we can use the actual and different objects that we created for the yuend() function. The ttextBF() is expecting only scores, not a tibble or data frame and the objects actual and different are tibbles. In both objects the scores are stored in a variable called recognition so we can specify the scores as different$recognition and actual$recognition. (Remember that $ means 'in', so different$recognition translates as 'the variable called recognition in the tibble called different'.

It's a good idea to save this model into a new object (lets call it calcite_bf) because you can do useful things with it (we didn't do this for the yuend() functions, but you could). For our data (using the default prior) we could execute:

calcite_bf <- BayesFactor::ttestBF(different$recognition, actual$recognition, paired = TRUE)
calcite_bf

The first line creates the object calcite_bf as described above and the second line prints it for us to see. Try this in the code box:

actual <- calcite_tidy %>%
  dplyr::filter(face_type == "actual") %>%
  dplyr::select(recognition)

different <- calcite_tidy %>%
  dplyr::filter(face_type == "different") %>%
  dplyr::select(recognition)

calcite_bf <- BayesFactor::ttestBF(different$recognition, actual$recognition, paired = TRUE)
calcite_bf

The value matches that in the book (an adventure in statistics). The Bayes factor is 1.48, which means that the probability of the data given the alternative hypothesis is 1.48 times greater than the probability of the data given the null hypothesis. Put another way, you should shift your belief in the alternative hypothesis relative to the null by a factor of 1.48 (i.e. not by much at all). The result favours the alternative hypothesis over the null by an amount that is ‘barely worth mentioning’.

Bayesian parameter estimates

We can get estimate of the parameters in the model using the posterior() function that we used in adventr_15 to extract the same information. The process is identical to what we did in that previous tutorial. We place the object we just created (calcite_bf) into the function and specify a number of iterations, then use the summary() function to view what we have created:

calcite_bf_est <- BayesFactor::posterior(calcite_bf, iterations = 10000)
summary(calcite_bf_est)

Try executing these two commands to view the estimates:

actual <- calcite_tidy %>%
  dplyr::filter(face_type == "actual") %>%
  dplyr::select(recognition)

different <- calcite_tidy %>%
  dplyr::filter(face_type == "different") %>%
  dplyr::select(recognition)

calcite_bf <- BayesFactor::ttestBF(different$recognition, actual$recognition, paired = TRUE)

calcite_bf_est <- BayesFactor::posterior(calcite_bf, iterations = 10000)
summary(calcite_bf_est)

The output will differ each time you run this (because it is based on a sampling process) so Figure 2 shows my output. The Bayesian estimate, assuming that the alternative hypothesis is true, of the difference between means (mu) is -2.09. You can use the 2.5% and 97.5% quantiles as the limits of the 95% credible interval for that difference. Again, assuming the alternative hypothesis is true, there is a 95% probability that the difference between means is somewhere between -4.298 and 0.056. Remember that you cannot use a credible interval to test hypotheses because it is constructed assuming that the alternative hypothesis is true. It tells you the interval within which the effect will fall with a 95% probability, assuming that the effect exists.

Figure 2: Bayesian estimates of the mask model

Other resources

Statistics

R

References



Try the adventr package in your browser

Any scripts or data that you put into this service are public.

adventr documentation built on July 1, 2020, 11:50 p.m.