This vignette illustrates some basic use cases of the mpt package, how to check for model identifiability, and how to perform simulation-based power analysis.
library(mpt)
The examples have been inspired by
Schmidt, O., Erdfelder, E., & Heck, D. W. [-@SchmidtErdfelder23]. How to develop, test, and extend multinomial processing tree models: A tutorial. Psychological Methods. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000561
@Bayen90 presented 40 younger and 40 older adults with a list of 50 words to be learned. The list consisted of 20 semantic word pairs and 10 singleton words. In a later memory test, participants freely recalled the presented words. For pairs, responses were classified into four categories: both words in a pair are recalled adjacently (E1) or non-adjacently (E2), one word in a pair is recalled (E3), neither word in a pair is recalled (E4); for singletons, into two categories: word recalled (F1), word not recalled (F2).
The individual recall frequencies are available in @SchmidtErdfelder23, see
?agememory
. Displayed below are the aggregate recall frequencies for
pairs and singletons by age group and lag condition.
dat <- read.table(header = TRUE, text = " lag age resp freq 0 young E1 90 # adjacent recall: apple, banana 0 young E2 14 # non-adjacent recall: apple, ..., banana 0 young E3 84 # single recall: only apple or only banana 0 young E4 212 # non-recall: neither apple nor banana n/a young F1 102 # recall n/a young F2 298 # non-recall 0 old E1 42 0 old E2 5 0 old E3 63 0 old E4 290 n/a old F1 64 n/a old F2 336 15 young E1 67 15 young E2 18 15 young E3 123 15 young E4 192 15 old E1 30 15 old E2 13 15 old E3 95 15 old E4 262 ") dat$age <- factor(dat$age, levels = c("young", "old"))
Specifying and fitting an MPT model involves two steps: mptspec()
specifies
the model, mpt()
fits it to the data.
For example, the storage-retrieval pair-clustering model for a single
condition consists of two multinomial category systems (trees): one for pairs,
one for singletons. The dot notation in mptspec()
can be used to identify
the trees.
# Pairs Singletons # # /r -- E1 # c # / \1-r -- E4 a -- F1 c: cluster storage # / / r: cluster retrieval # \ /u -- E2 \ u: non-clustered storage/retrieval # \ u 1-a -- F2 a: singleton storage/retrieval # \ / \1-u -- E3 # 1-c # \ /u -- E3 # 1-u # \1-u -- E4 mptspec( # specify model E.1 = c*r, E.2 = (1 - c)*u^2, # dot notation: E.3 = 2 * (1 - c)*u*(1 - u), # tree.response E.4 = c*(1 - r) + (1 - c)*(1 - u)^2, F.1 = a, F.2 = 1 - a, .restr = list(a = u) ) |> mpt(data = dat[dat$lag != 15 & dat$age == "young", ]) # fit to data
Alternatively, some model structures are pre-specified and can be called by
passing a string label, see ?mptspec
.
m <- mpt(mptspec("SR"), data = dat[dat$lag != 15 & dat$age == "young", ])
Standard extractor functions work on the model object.
print(m) summary(m) logLik(m) AIC(m) BIC(m) coef(m) confint(m, logit = FALSE)
(a) Use mptspec()
to specify the one-high-threshold model:
# Target Distractor # # r -- Hit # / } 55 b -- FA 45 # / b -- Hit / r: recognition # \ / \ b: bias # 1-r 1-b -- CR 765 # \ # 1-b -- Miss 35
(b) Use mpt()
to fit the model to the response frequencies [taken from
@BroederSchuetz09, see ?recogROC
].
There are two parameter estimation methods available in mpt()
. One is an
application of the EM algorithm [@Hu99]. The other (the default) uses
optim()
's BFGS method with analytic gradients.
A simplified version of the latter is illustrated here. It requires two parts. First, a function that returns the negative log-likelihood of the MPT model.
nll <- function(theta, data, treeid) { # negative log-likelihood c <- theta[1] r <- theta[2] u <- theta[3] a <- theta[3] # a = u pcat <- c( E.1 = c*r, E.2 = (1 - c)*u^2, E.3 = 2 * (1 - c)*u*(1 - u), E.4 = c*(1 - r) + (1 - c)*(1 - u)^2, F.1 = a, F.2 = 1 - a ) -sum( dmultinom(data[treeid == 1], size = sum(data[treeid == 1]), prob = pcat[treeid == 1], log = TRUE), dmultinom(data[treeid == 2], size = sum(data[treeid == 2]), prob = pcat[treeid == 2], log = TRUE) ) }
Second, a call to optim()
passing the function as an argument.
optim(par = c(.5, .5, .5), # starting values fn = nll, # function to be minimized data = dat[dat$lag != 15 & dat$age == "young", "freq"], # arguments passed to fn treeid = c(1, 1, 1, 1, 2, 2), method = "L-BFGS-B", # algorithm lower = rep(.001, 3), # boundaries upper = rep(.999, 3), hessian = TRUE)
(a) Write an R function that returns the negative log-likelihood of the one-high-threshold model.
(b) Use optim()
to estimate the parameters for the example data from before.
(c) Compare the results to the output of mpt()
.
Model comparison is a two-step procedure. First, specify and fit a restricted model, where the restriction corresponds to the hypothesis being tested (here, H~0~: $c = 0.5$).
m0 <- update(m$spec, .restr = list(c = 0.5)) |> # specify restriction(s) mpt(data = m$y) |> # refit print()
Second, test the hypothesis by comparing the likelihoods of the restricted and the unrestricted models.
anova(m0, m)
(a) Fit a storage-retrieval pair-clustering model that jointly accounts for young and old participants (ignoring the lag-15 pairs).
(b) Test the age effect on - the $c$ parameter - the $r$ parameter.
(c) Discuss the results.
Consider the storage-retrieval pair-clustering model for young and old participants in its original parameterization.
m1 <- mptspec("SR", .replicates = 2) |> mpt(data = dat[dat$lag != 15, ]) |> print()
An alternative parameterization substitutes $c_{old} = s \cdot c_{young}$, where $s \in (0, 1)$ is a shrinkage parameter that enforces the order constraint $c_{old} \leq c_{young}$.
update(m1$spec, .restr = list(c2 = s * c1)) |> mpt(data = m1$y)
The idea of shrinkage parameters is useful for testing interaction hypotheses:
H~0~: Age decline in $c$ and $r$ parameters is equally strong (no interaction between memory process and age), $s_c = s_r$.
m5 <- update(m1$spec, .restr = list(c2 = s_c * c1, # two shrinkage pars r2 = s_r * r1)) |> mpt(data = m1$y) m6 <- update(m5$spec, .restr = list(s_c = s_r)) |> # single shrinkage par mpt(data = m1$y) anova(m6, m5)
(a) Fit a storage-retrieval pair-clustering model that jointly accounts for all observations: - young and old participants - lag-0 pairs, singletons, lag-15 pairs.
The resulting model has four sets of $c$, $r$, and $u$ parameters (but be careful: singletons have no lag!).
(b) Refine the previous model such that it includes only a single $u_{young}$ and a single $u_{old}$ parameter.
(c) Reparameterize the refined model to account for an age-decline effect on the $r$ parameters. Include - a shrinkage parameter for lag-0 pairs - a shrinkage parameter for lag-15 pairs.
(d) Test whether age decline is equally strong in both lag conditions. Discuss the results.
A model is locally identifiable if its prediction function in the neighborhood of some parameter values is one to one, so that its Jacobian (the matrix of partial derivatives) will have full rank.
In the storage-retrieval pair-clustering model, one might assume that processing of non-clustered words in a pair differs for the first and the second word. This implies two different parameters ($u_1$ and $u_2$) and thus renders the model non-identifiable.
s1 <- mptspec( # not identifiable: 5 parameters E.1 = c * r, E.2 = (1 - c) * u1 * u2, E.3 = (1 - c) * u1 * (1 - u2) + (1 - c) * u2 * (1 - u1), E.4 = c * (1 - r) + (1 - c) * (1 - u1) * (1 - u2), F.1 = a, F.2 = 1 - a )
Its Jacobian at random parameter location is rank deficient: the rank is less than its number of parameters.
runif(5) |> s1$par2deriv() |> _$deriv |> print() |> qr() |> _$rank
Setting $u_1 = u_2$ makes the model identifiable. Its Jacobian has full rank.
s2 <- update(s1, .restr = list(u1 = u, u2 = u)) # make it identifiable runif(4) |> s2$par2deriv() |> _$deriv |> print() |> qr() |> _$rank
Another quick check of local identifiability is to re-estimate the model from random starting values. If the model converges to the same maximum but has non-unique estimates, it possibly is not identifiable.
suppressWarnings(replicate(10, mpt(s1, data = dat[dat$lag != 15 & dat$age == "young", ], start = runif(5)) |> coef() )) |> t() # not identifiable: parameter trade-offs replicate(10, mpt(s2, data = dat[dat$lag != 15 & dat$age == "young", ], start = runif(4)) |> coef() ) |> t() # identifiable: unique estimates
(a) Specify a one-high-threshold model that is limited to target items only and check its identifiability by - computing the rank of the Jacobian - re-estimating the parameters from random starting values.
(b) Fix the bias parameter to, e.g., $b = 0.2$ and redo the identifiability checks. What do you notice?
Simulation-based power analysis involves four steps [e.g., @Wickelmaier22]: specifying a model that includes the effect of interest, generating data from the model, testing the null hypothesis, repeating data generation and testing. The proportion of times the test turns out significant is an estimate of its power.
Set up a data generator.
s <- mptspec( E.1 = c * r, E.2 = (1 - c) * u^2, E.3 = 2 * (1 - c) * u * (1 - u), E.4 = c * (1 - r) + (1 - c) * (1 - u)^2, F.1 = a, F.2 = 1 - a ) s$par # before you use par2prob(), carefully check position of parameters! s$par2prob(c(c = 0.5, r = 0.5, u = 0.4, a = 0.6)) # evaluate model equations dataGen <- function(nn, d) { structure(list( # stub mpt object treeid = s$treeid, n = setNames((nn * c(2, 1)/3)[s$treeid], s$treeid), # 2:1 ratio pcat = s$par2prob(c(c = 0.5, r = 0.5, u = 0.5 - d/2, a = 0.5 + d/2)) ), class = "mpt") |> simulate() } dataGen(960, 0.2)
Try to recover the parameter values from the generated responses.
replicate(20, dataGen(960, 0.2) |> mpt(s, data = _) |> coef()) |> t() |> boxplot(horizontal = TRUE)
Run your own parameter recovery:
Did you succeed in recovering the parameters?
The first example is a power analysis of the goodness-of-fit test of the storage-retrieval pair-clustering model. The model assumes identical processing of non-clustered pairs and singletons, so H~0~: $a = u$.
Set up a function that calls the data generator, performs the test, and returns its p value.
testFun <- function(nn, d) { y <- dataGen(nn, d) # generate data with effect m1 <- mpt(s, y) m2 <- mpt(update(s, .restr = list(a = u)), y) anova(m2, m1)$"Pr(>Chi)"[2] # test H0, return p value }
Repeating the data generation and testing steps for each condition may take some time, depending on the model, the number of conditions, and the number of replications.
# pwrsim1 <- expand.grid(d = seq(0, 0.5, 0.1), n = 30 * 2^(0:5)) # # system.time( # about 20 min # pwrsim1$pval <- # mapply(function(nn, d) replicate(500, testFun(nn, d)), # nn = pwrsim1$n, d = pwrsim1$d, SIMPLIFY = FALSE) # ) # pwrsim1$pwr <- sapply(pwrsim1$pval, function(p) mean(p < .05)) # }
To save a bit of time, the above simulation has been pre-run, see ?pwrsim
,
and results are reused here for illustration.
data(pwrsim) # provides pwrsim1 and pwrsim2 if(requireNamespace("lattice")) { lattice::xyplot(pwr ~ d, pwrsim1, groups = n, type = c("g", "b"), auto.key = list(space = "right"), xlab = "Parameter difference a - u", ylab = "Simulated power") }
The second example is a power analysis of a test of age differences in retrieval. It requires a two-group model, so the hypothesis $r_{young} = r_{old}$ can be tested.
s <- mptspec("SR", .replicates = 2) dataGen <- function(nn, d) { structure(list( treeid = s$treeid, n = setNames((nn/2 * c(2, 1, 2, 1)/3)[s$treeid], s$treeid), pcat = s$par2prob(c(c1 = 0.5, r1 = 0.4 + d/2, u1 = 0.3, # young c2 = 0.5, r2 = 0.4 - d/2, u2 = 0.3)) # old ), class = "mpt") |> simulate(m) } testFun <- function(nn, d) { y <- dataGen(nn, d) m1 <- mpt(s, y) m2 <- mpt(update(s, .restr = list(r1 = r2)), y) anova(m2, m1)$"Pr(>Chi)"[2] } # pwrsim2 <- expand.grid(d = seq(0, 0.4, 0.1), n = 120 * 2^(0:2)) if(requireNamespace("lattice")) { lattice::xyplot(pwr ~ d, pwrsim2, groups = n, type = c("g", "b"), auto.key = list(space = "right"), xlab = "Parameter difference r1 - r2", ylab = "Simulated power") }
In the storage-retrieval pair-clustering model, what sample size is required to detect a parameter difference $r_{young} - r_{old} = 0.4$ with a power of about 0.8?
Simulate power for
Any scripts or data that you put into this service are public.
Add the following code to your website.
For more information on customizing the embed code, read Embedding Snippets.