mptspec | R Documentation |
Returns the specification of an MPT model object for fitting with
mpt
.
mptspec(..., .replicates = NULL, .restr = NULL)
## S3 method for class 'mptspec'
update(object, .replicates = NULL, .restr = NULL, ...)
... |
(named) expressions or a character string specifying the model. See Details. |
.replicates |
the number of replicates of the model equations. See Details. |
.restr |
a named list of parameter restrictions. See Details. |
object |
an object of class |
...
is used to symbolically specify the MPT model equations by
suitable expressions, for example, they could look like this
r + (1 - r)*b, (1 - r)*(1 - b), b, 1 - b
where each expression represents the probability of a response in the corresponding category (link probabilities are multiplied, branch probabilities are added). Thus, there usually are as many expressions as response categories.
Joint (or product) multinomial models consist of more than a single
processing tree. To identify the trees in such a model, expressions may
have optional names. Canonically, these names are of the form x.y
,
where x
is the tree identifier (treeid
) and y
specifies
the response category within a tree.
Alternatively, ...
may be a character string identifying one out of
a list of pre-specified MPT models. Currently accessible are the following
models (other models have to be specified by explicit expressions as
described above):
1HT
: the one-high-threshold model (Blackwell, 1963; Swets, 1961).
2HT
: the two-high-threshold model (Snodgrass & Corwin, 1988; see also
Broeder & Schuetz, 2009).
PairAsso
: the paired-associate learning model (Riefer & Batchelder,
1988).
proCNI
: the CNI model of moral dilemma judgment for proscriptive
norms (Hennig & Huetter, 2020). The general formula includes the process
dissociation (PD) model (Conway & Gawronski, 2013) as a special case.
prospec
: the event-based prospective memory model (Smith & Bayen,
2004).
rmodel
: the r-model of recognition heuristic use (Hilbig, Erdfelder,
& Pohl, 2010).
SourceMon
: the source-monitoring model (Batchelder & Riefer, 1990).
SR
, SR2
: the storage-retrieval pair-clustering model
(Batchelder & Riefer, 1986). SR2
is the model without singleton
items.
WST
: the inference-guessing model with relaxed assumptions (Klauer,
Stahl, & Erdfelder, 2007) for the Wason selection task.
The intended use of .replicates
is to specify the number of
replicates of the model equations, for example, when the same model is
repeatedly applied in several experimental conditions. Accordingly,
parameter names are augmented by numbers to make them unique.
Parameter restrictions included in .restr
may be of the form
b = r
or b = 0.5
etc. Depending on the fitting algorithm
employed in mpt
(BFGS
, but not EM
), mathematical
functions are permissible, for example, b = sqrt(r)
.
The update
method is used to add parameter restrictions or replicates
to an existing mptspec
object.
An object of class mptspec
that serves as input to mpt
which fits the model to data. It consists of the following components:
par2prob |
a function that takes a vector of parameter values and computes the response probabilities. |
par2deriv |
a function that takes a vector of parameter values and computes first and second derivatives of the model equations. |
prob |
a list containing expressions of the model equations. |
deriv |
a list containing expressions of the first and second derivatives of the model equations. |
par |
a named vector of parameter values. |
replicates |
the number of replicates of the model equations. |
restr |
a list containing expressions of parameter restrictions. |
treeid |
a factor that identifies each tree. |
Batchelder, W.H., & Riefer, D.M. (1986). The statistical analysis of a model for storage and retrieval processes in human memory. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 39(2), 129–149. \Sexpr[results=rd]{tools:::Rd_expr_doi("10.1111/j.2044-8317.1986.tb00852.x")}
Batchelder, W.H., & Riefer, D.M. (1990). Multinomial processing models of source monitoring. Psychological Review, 97(4), 548–564. \Sexpr[results=rd]{tools:::Rd_expr_doi("10.1037/0033-295x.97.4.548")}
Blackwell, H.R. (1963). Neural theories of simple visual discriminations. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 53(1), 129–160. \Sexpr[results=rd]{tools:::Rd_expr_doi("10.1364/JOSA.53.000129")}
Broeder, A., & Schuetz, J. (2009). Recognition ROCs are curvilinear–or are they? On premature arguments against the two-high-threshold model of recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(3), 587–606. \Sexpr[results=rd]{tools:::Rd_expr_doi("10.1037/a0015279")}
Conway, P., & Gawronski, B. (2013). Deontological and utilitarian inclinations in moral decision making: A process dissociation approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(2), 216–235. \Sexpr[results=rd]{tools:::Rd_expr_doi("10.1037/a0031021")}
Hennig, M., & Huetter, M. (2020). Revisiting the divide between deontology and utilitarianism in moral dilemma judgment: A multinomial modeling approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 118(1), 22–56. \Sexpr[results=rd]{tools:::Rd_expr_doi("10.1037/pspa0000173")}
Hilbig, B.E., Erdfelder, E., & Pohl, R.F. (2010). One-reason decision-making unveiled: A measurement model of the recognition heuristic. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(1), 123–134. \Sexpr[results=rd]{tools:::Rd_expr_doi("10.1037/a0017518")}
Klauer, K.C., Stahl, C., & Erdfelder, E. (2007). The abstract selection task: New data and an almost comprehensive model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(4), 680–703. \Sexpr[results=rd]{tools:::Rd_expr_doi("10.1037/0278-7393.33.4.680")}
Riefer, D.M., & Batchelder, W.H. (1988). Multinomial modeling and the measurement of cognitive processes. Psychological Review, 95(3), 318–339. \Sexpr[results=rd]{tools:::Rd_expr_doi("10.1037/0033-295x.95.3.318")}
Smith, R.E., & Bayen, U.J. (2004). A multinomial model of event-based prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30(4), 756–777. \Sexpr[results=rd]{tools:::Rd_expr_doi("10.1037/0278-7393.30.4.756")}
Snodgrass, J.G., & Corwin, J. (1988). Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Applications to dementia and amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117(1), 34–50. \Sexpr[results=rd]{tools:::Rd_expr_doi("10.1037/0096-3445.117.1.34")}
Swets, J. A. (1961). Is there a sensory threshold? Science, 134(3473), 168–177. \Sexpr[results=rd]{tools:::Rd_expr_doi("10.1126/science.134.3473.168")}
mpt
.
## Specify storage-retrieval pair-clustering model for pairs
spec1 <- mptspec(
c*r,
(1 - c)*u^2,
2*(1 - c)*u*(1 - u),
c*(1 - r) + (1 - c)*(1 - u)^2
)
## Specify model with parameter restrictions
spec2 <- mptspec(
c*r,
(1 - c)*u^2,
2*(1 - c)*u*(1 - u),
c*(1 - r) + (1 - c)*(1 - u)^2,
.restr = list(c = r/2, u = 0.3)
)
## Optional names (dot notation) identifying trees in joint MPT model
spec3 <- mptspec(
Target.Hit = r + (1 - r)*b,
Target.Miss = (1 - r)*(1 - b),
Distractor.FA = b,
Distractor.CR = 1 - b,
.replicates = 3,
.restr = list(r1 = r, r2 = r, r3 = r)
)
## Pre-specified one-high-threshold model
spec4 <- mptspec("1HT")
## Fit to data in Broeder and Schuetz (2009)
m <- mpt(spec4, c(55, 35, 45, 765))
## Working with the mptspec object
spec4$par2prob(c(0.5, 0.1)) # response probabilities
spec4$par2deriv(coef(m))$deriv # Jacobian matrix at ML estimate
## See data(package = "mpt") for application examples.
Add the following code to your website.
For more information on customizing the embed code, read Embedding Snippets.