IntroAnalysis:::ma223_setup() library(learnr) knitr::opts_chunk$set(echo = FALSE) learnr::tutorial_options(exercise.cap = "Exercise")
```{css, echo=FALSE} / Solution Functionality / .accordion > input[type="checkbox"] { position: absolute; left: -100vw; }
.accordion .content { overflow-y: hidden; height: 0; transition: height 0.3s ease; }
.accordion > input[type="checkbox"]:checked ~ .content { height: auto; overflow: visible; }
.accordion label { display: block; }
/ Styling /
.accordion { margin-bottom: 1em; }
.accordion > input[type="checkbox"]:checked ~ .content { padding: 15px; border: 1px solid #e8e8e8; border-top: 0; }
.accordion .handle { margin: 0; }
.accordion label { cursor: pointer; font-weight: normal; padding: 15px; }
.accordion label:hover, .accordion label:focus { background: #d8d8d8; }
/ Turning arrow / .accordion .handle label:before { font-family: 'fontawesome'; content: "\f054"; display: inline-block; margin-right: 10px; font-size: .58em; line-height: 1.556em; vertical-align: middle; }
.accordion > input[type="checkbox"]:checked ~ .handle label:before { content: "\f078"; }
.tipbox { padding: 1em 1em 1em 1em; border: 1px solid black; border-radius: 10px; box-shadow: 2px 2px #888888; }
.keyidea { border: 1px solid #54585A; background: #4F758B; color: white; }
.keyidea:before { content: "Key Idea: "; font-weight: bold; }
.tip { background: lightyellow; }
.tip:before { content: "Tip: "; font-weight: bold; }
.title{ color: #800000; background: none; }
.solution { background: #ebebeb; }
h2{ color: white; background-color: #800000; }
h3{ color: #800000; border-bottom: thick solid #696969; }
h4{ color: #800000; }
blockquote { font-size: inherit; padding: 0px 20px; }
## Background A [recent study](https://projects.thepostathens.com/SpecialProjects/behind-the-screens-time-spent-on-phone/index.html) suggested college students spend an average of 4 hours and 25 minutes on their phone each day. [Another study](https://psychcentral.com/news/2014/08/31/new-study-finds-cell-phone-addiction-increasingly-realistic-possibility#1) suggests approximately 6 out of 10 students believe they are addicted to their cell phone. For faculty, cell phones in class are a constant source of distraction. Anecdotal, faculty believe students who regularly check their cell phone in class tend to perform more poorly on assessments. Some faculty have considered banning cell phones from class, but given the attachment we have to mobile devices, could banning cell phones have a negative impact on students? > There is no denying that mobile devices (whether cell phones or smart watches) are constantly vying for our attention. In addition to staying in contact with loved ones, app notifications want us to engage with their product (which is what makes them profitable). There is no such thing as multitasking; what we perceive as "doing two things at once" is really our brain switching attention between multiple tasks repeatedly. Every time we are pulled back to our device, our brain has to reset as it switches attention from one task to another; this diminishes our focus on any task. If you are interested in learning more tools to help you reduce the distraction of mobile devices, you should speak with the office of [Student Academic Success.](https://rosehulman.sharepoint.com/sites/osas/SitePages/Home.aspx) A sample of 144 adults recruited through a Psychology Department at Florida Gulf Coast University participated in a study designed to investigate the impact of having limited access to their mobile device. Each participant entered a room and took an assessment of their positive and negative affect (emotional state). Then, the participant was randomly assigned to one of three groups/conditions. The first group was told to "keep your cell phone on but we ask that you put it away for the remainder of the experiment" ("On" group). The second group was told to "turn your cell phone off and put it away for the remainder of the experiment" ("Off" group). The third group was told to "turn your cell phone off, I need to remove it for the remainder of the session, I will keep it in a safe location and once the study is complete I will return it to you" ("Removed" group). After being assigned to a condition, participants repeated the assessment of their affect. At this point, participants were asked to wait in the room alone until the researcher returned. The researcher returned exactly 3 minutes later, at which time the participants were asked to answer the following question: "Precisely how long would you estimate the researcher was just out of the room? Please mark your estimate and try to be exact." Participants marked their answer on a scale ranging from 0 to 10 minutes. The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) was used to assess positive and negative affect (emotional state). This assessment consists of 20-items, each marked on a 5-point scale. The responses on ten items are combined to measure positive affect (e.g., interested, excited, alert), and the responses on ten items are combined to measure negative affect (e.g., distressed, upset, guilty). We have the data corresponding to this study (`cellphones`). We are primarily interested in seeing how the group to which the participants was assigned (`Condition`) impacts how long they perceived they were left alone (`Duration`). This is addressed by the following model: $$(\text{Duration})_i = \mu_1 (\text{Off})_i + \mu_2 (\text{On})_i + \mu_3 (\text{Removed})_i + \varepsilon_i$$ where $$ \begin{aligned} (\text{Off})_i &= \begin{cases} 1 & \text{if i-th participant assigned to the "Off" group} \\ 0 & \text{otherwise} \end{cases} \\ (\text{On})_i &= \begin{cases} 1 & \text{if i-th participant assigned to the "On" group} \\ 0 & \text{otherwise} \end{cases} \\ (\text{Removed})_i &= \begin{cases} 1 & \text{if i-th participant assigned to the "Removed" group} \\ 0 & \text{otherwise} \end{cases} \\ \end{aligned} $$ are indicator variables capturing group assignment. The data is summarized below: ```r ggplot(data = cellphones) + aes(y = Duration, x = Condition) + labs(y = 'Perceived Length of Time\nLeft Alone (minutes)', x = 'Cell Phone Group Assigned') + geom_boxplot() + geom_jitter()
The above model can be fit using the following code.
cellphone.model = specify_mean_model(Duration ~ Condition, data = cellphones)
cellphone.model = specify_mean_model(Duration ~ Condition, data = cellphones)
Obtain the residuals and fitted values for the above model, and store them in a dataset called
cellphones.diag
. What does the residual for each observation tell you?
cellphones.diag = obtain_diagnostics()
cellphones.diag = obtain_diagnostics(cellphone.model)
cellphone.model = specify_mean_model(Duration ~ Condition, data = cellphones) cellphones.diag = obtain_diagnostics(cellphone.model)
Is it reasonable to assume the error in the above model comes from a Normal distribution? Explain.
ggplot() + aes() + labs()
ggplot(data = cellphones.diag) + aes(sample = .resid) + labs(y = "Sample Quantiles", x = "Theoretical Quantiles") + geom_qq()
Is it reasonable that the variability in the error of the perceived duration is constant across all three treatment groups? Explain.
ggplot() + aes() + labs()
ggplot(data = cellphones.diag) + aes(y = .resid, x = Condition) + labs(y = "Residuals", x = "Cell Phone Group Assigned") + geom_boxplot() + geom_jitter()
The unique participant identification code suggests the data is presented in the order in which it was collected. Is it reasonable to assume the error in the perceived duration for one individual is independent of the error in the perceived duration for any other individual? Explain.
ggplot() + aes() + labs()
ggplot(data = cellphones.diag) + aes(y = .resid, x = seq_along(.resid)) + labs(y = "Residuals", x = "Order of Observations") + geom_point() + geom_line()
Explain why we do not need to assess the "mean-0" condition.
Add the following code to your website.
For more information on customizing the embed code, read Embedding Snippets.