stopifnot(require(knitr)) options(width = 90) opts_chunk$set( comment = NA, message = FALSE, warning = FALSE, eval = if (isTRUE(exists("params"))) params$EVAL else FALSE, dev = "jpeg", dpi = 100, fig.asp = 0.8, fig.width = 5, out.width = "60%", fig.align = "center" ) library(brms) ggplot2::theme_set(theme_default())
This vignette provides an introduction on how to fit non-linear multilevel models with brms. Non-linear models are incredibly flexible and powerful, but require much more care with respect to model specification and priors than typical generalized linear models. Ignoring group-level effects for the moment, the predictor term $\eta_n$ of a generalized linear model for observation $n$ can be written as follows:
$$\eta_n = \sum_{i = 1}^K b_i x_{ni}$$
where $b_i$ is the regression coefficient of predictor $i$ and $x_{ni}$ is the data of predictor $i$ for observation $n$. This also comprises interaction terms and various other data transformations. However, the structure of $\eta_n$ is always linear in the sense that the regression coefficients $b_i$ are multiplied by some predictor values and then summed up. This implies that the hypothetical predictor term
$$\eta_n = b_1 \exp(b_2 x_n)$$
would not be a linear predictor anymore and we could not fit it using
classical techniques of generalized linear models. We thus need a more general
model class, which we will call non-linear models. Note that the term
'non-linear' does not say anything about the assumed distribution of the
response variable. In particular it does not mean 'not normally distributed' as
we can apply non-linear predictor terms to all kinds of response distributions
(for more details on response distributions available in brms see
vignette("brms_families")
).
We begin with a simple example using simulated data.
b <- c(2, 0.75) x <- rnorm(100) y <- rnorm(100, mean = b[1] * exp(b[2] * x)) dat1 <- data.frame(x, y)
As stated above, we cannot use a generalized linear model to estimate $b$ so we go ahead an specify a non-linear model.
prior1 <- prior(normal(1, 2), nlpar = "b1") + prior(normal(0, 2), nlpar = "b2") fit1 <- brm(bf(y ~ b1 * exp(b2 * x), b1 + b2 ~ 1, nl = TRUE), data = dat1, prior = prior1)
When looking at the above code, the first thing that becomes obvious is that we
changed the formula
syntax to display the non-linear formula including
predictors (i.e., x
) and parameters (i.e., b1
and b2
) wrapped in a call to
bf
. This stands in contrast to classical R formulas, where only predictors
are given and parameters are implicit. The argument b1 + b2 ~ 1
serves two
purposes. First, it provides information, which variables in formula
are
parameters, and second, it specifies the linear predictor terms for each
parameter. In fact, we should think of non-linear parameters as placeholders for
linear predictor terms rather than as parameters themselves (see also the
following examples). In the present case, we have no further variables to
predict b1
and b2
and thus we just fit intercepts that represent our
estimates of $b_1$ and $b_2$ in the model equation above. The formula b1 + b2 ~
1
is a short form of b1 ~ 1, b2 ~ 1
that can be used if multiple non-linear
parameters share the same formula. Setting nl = TRUE
tells brms that the
formula should be treated as non-linear.
In contrast to generalized linear models, priors on population-level parameters
(i.e., 'fixed effects') are often mandatory to identify a non-linear model.
Thus, brms requires the user to explicitly specify these priors. In the
present example, we used a normal(1, 2)
prior on (the population-level
intercept of) b1
, while we used a normal(0, 2)
prior on (the
population-level intercept of) b2
. Setting priors is a non-trivial task in all
kinds of models, especially in non-linear models, so you should always invest
some time to think of appropriate priors. Quite often, you may be forced to
change your priors after fitting a non-linear model for the first time, when you
observe different MCMC chains converging to different posterior regions. This is
a clear sign of an identification problem and one solution is to set stronger
(i.e., more narrow) priors.
To obtain summaries of the fitted model, we apply
summary(fit1) plot(fit1) plot(conditional_effects(fit1), points = TRUE)
The summary
method reveals that we were able to recover the true parameter
values pretty nicely. According to the plot
method, our MCMC chains have
converged well and to the same posterior. The conditional_effects
method
visualizes the model-implied (non-linear) regression line.
We might be also interested in comparing our non-linear model to a classical linear model.
fit2 <- brm(y ~ x, data = dat1)
summary(fit2)
To investigate and compare model fit, we can apply graphical posterior predictive checks, which make use of the bayesplot package on the backend.
pp_check(fit1) pp_check(fit2)
We can also easily compare model fit using leave-one-out cross-validation.
loo(fit1, fit2)
Since smaller LOOIC
values indicate better model fit, it is immediately
evident that the non-linear model fits the data better, which is of course not
too surprising since we simulated the data from exactly that model.
On his blog, Markus Gesmann predicts the growth of cumulative insurance loss payments over time, originated from different origin years (see https://www.magesblog.com/post/2015-11-03-loss-developments-via-growth-curves-and/). We will use a slightly simplified version of his model for demonstration purposes here. It looks as follows:
$$cum_{AY, dev} \sim N(\mu_{AY, dev}, \sigma)$$ $$\mu_{AY, dev} = ult_{AY} \left(1 - \exp\left(- \left( \frac{dev}{\theta} \right)^\omega \right) \right)$$
The cumulative insurance payments $cum$ will grow over time, and we model this dependency using the variable $dev$. Further, $ult_{AY}$ is the (to be estimated) ultimate loss of accident each year. It constitutes a non-linear parameter in our framework along with the parameters $\theta$ and $\omega$, which are responsible for the growth of the cumulative loss and are assumed to be the same across years. The data is already shipped with brms.
data(loss) head(loss)
and translate the proposed model into a non-linear brms model.
fit_loss <- brm( bf(cum ~ ult * (1 - exp(-(dev/theta)^omega)), ult ~ 1 + (1|AY), omega ~ 1, theta ~ 1, nl = TRUE), data = loss, family = gaussian(), prior = c( prior(normal(5000, 1000), nlpar = "ult"), prior(normal(1, 2), nlpar = "omega"), prior(normal(45, 10), nlpar = "theta") ), control = list(adapt_delta = 0.9) )
We estimate a group-level effect of accident year (variable AY
) for the
ultimate loss ult
. This also shows nicely how a non-linear parameter is
actually a placeholder for a linear predictor, which in case of ult
, contains
only an varying intercept over year. Again, priors on population-level effects
are required and, for the present model, are actually mandatory to ensure
identifiability. We summarize the model using well known methods.
summary(fit_loss) plot(fit_loss, N = 3, ask = FALSE) conditional_effects(fit_loss)
Next, we show marginal effects separately for each year.
conditions <- data.frame(AY = unique(loss$AY)) rownames(conditions) <- unique(loss$AY) me_loss <- conditional_effects( fit_loss, conditions = conditions, re_formula = NULL, method = "predict" ) plot(me_loss, ncol = 5, points = TRUE)
It is evident that there is some variation in cumulative loss across accident years, for instance due to natural disasters happening only in certain years. Further, we see that the uncertainty in the predicted cumulative loss is larger for later years with fewer available data points. For a more detailed discussion of this data set, see Section 4.5 in Gesmann & Morris (2020).
As a third example, we want to show how to model more advanced item-response models using the non-linear model framework of brms. For simplicity, suppose we have a single forced choice item with three alternatives of which only one is correct. Our response variable is whether a person answers the item correctly (1) or not (0). Person are assumed to vary in their ability to answer the item correctly. However, every person has a 33% chance of getting the item right just by guessing. We thus simulate some data to reflect this situation.
inv_logit <- function(x) 1 / (1 + exp(-x)) ability <- rnorm(300) p <- 0.33 + 0.67 * inv_logit(ability) answer <- ifelse(runif(300, 0, 1) < p, 1, 0) dat_ir <- data.frame(ability, answer)
The most basic item-response model is equivalent to a simple logistic regression model.
fit_ir1 <- brm(answer ~ ability, data = dat_ir, family = bernoulli())
However, this model completely ignores the guessing probability and will thus likely come to biased estimates and predictions.
summary(fit_ir1) plot(conditional_effects(fit_ir1), points = TRUE)
A more sophisticated approach incorporating the guessing probability looks as follows:
fit_ir2 <- brm( bf(answer ~ 0.33 + 0.67 * inv_logit(eta), eta ~ ability, nl = TRUE), data = dat_ir, family = bernoulli("identity"), prior = prior(normal(0, 5), nlpar = "eta") )
It is very important to set the link function of the bernoulli
family to
identity
or else we will apply two link functions. This is because our
non-linear predictor term already contains the desired link function (0.33 +
0.67 * inv_logit
), but the bernoulli
family applies the default logit
link
on top of it. This will of course lead to strange and uninterpretable results.
Thus, please make sure that you set the link function to identity
, whenever
your non-linear predictor term already contains the desired link function.
summary(fit_ir2) plot(conditional_effects(fit_ir2), points = TRUE)
Comparing model fit via leave-one-out cross-validation
loo(fit_ir1, fit_ir2)
shows that both model fit the data equally well, but remember that predictions of the first model might still be misleading as they may well be below the guessing probability for low ability values. Now, suppose that we don't know the guessing probability and want to estimate it from the data. This can easily be done changing the previous model just a bit.
fit_ir3 <- brm( bf(answer ~ guess + (1 - guess) * inv_logit(eta), eta ~ 0 + ability, guess ~ 1, nl = TRUE), data = dat_ir, family = bernoulli("identity"), prior = c( prior(normal(0, 5), nlpar = "eta"), prior(beta(1, 1), nlpar = "guess", lb = 0, ub = 1) ) )
Here, we model the guessing probability as a non-linear parameter making sure
that it cannot exceed the interval $[0, 1]$. We did not estimate an intercept
for eta
, as this will lead to a bias in the estimated guessing parameter (try
it out; this is an excellent example of how careful one has to be in non-linear
models).
summary(fit_ir3) plot(fit_ir3) plot(conditional_effects(fit_ir3), points = TRUE)
The results show that we are able to recover the simulated model parameters with this non-linear model. Of course, real item-response data have multiple items so that accounting for item and person variability (e.g., using a multilevel model with varying intercepts) becomes necessary as we have multiple observations per item and person. Luckily, this can all be done within the non-linear framework of brms and I hope that this vignette serves as a good starting point.
Gesmann M. & Morris J. (2020). Hierarchical Compartmental Reserving Models. CAS Research Papers.
Any scripts or data that you put into this service are public.
Add the following code to your website.
For more information on customizing the embed code, read Embedding Snippets.