slpMack75: Mackintosh (1975) associative learning model

View source: R/RcppExports.R

slpMack75R Documentation

Mackintosh (1975) associative learning model

Description

Mackintosh's (1975) attentional learning model, as implemented by Le Pelley et al. (2016).

Usage


slpMack75(st, tr, xtdo = FALSE)

Arguments

st

List of model parameters

tr

Matrix of training items

xtdo

Boolean specifying whether to include extended information in the output (see below)

Details

The function operates as a stateful list processor (slp; see Wills et al., 2017). Specifically, it takes a matrix (tr) as an argument, where each row represents a single training trial, while each column represents the different types of information required by the model, such as the elemental representation of the training stimuli, and the presence or absence of an outcome. It returns the output activation on each trial (a.k.a. sum of associative strengths of cues present on that trial), as a vector. The slpMack75 function also returns the final state of the model - a vector of associative and attentional strengths between each stimulus and the outcome representation.

Argument st must be a list containing the following items:

lr - the associative learning rate (fixed for a given simulation), as denoted by theta in Equation 1 of Mackintosh (1975).

alr - the attentional learning rate parameter. It can be set without limit (see alpha below), but we recommend setting this parameter to somewhere between 0.1 and 1.

w - a vector of initial associative strengths. If you are not sure what to use here, set all values to zero.

alpha - a vector of initial attentional strengths. If the updated value is above 1 or below 0.1, it is capped to 1 and 0.1 respectively.

colskip - the number of optional columns to be skipped in the tr matrix. colskip should be set to the number of optional columns you have added to the tr matrix, PLUS ONE. So, if you have added no optional columns, colskip=1. This is because the first (non-optional) column contains the control values (details below).

Argument tr must be a matrix, where each row is one trial presented to the model. Trials are always presented in the order specified. The columns must be as described below, in the order described below:

ctrl - a vector of control codes. Available codes are:

0 = normal trial 1 = reset model (i.e. set associative strengths back to their initial values as specified in w) 2 = Freeze learning 3 = Reset associative weights to initial state, but keep attentional strengths in alpha 4 = Reset attentional strengths to initial state, but keep association weights.

Control codes are actioned before the trial is processed.

opt1, opt2, ... - any number of preferred optional columns, the names of which can be chosen by the user. It is important that these columns are placed after the control column, and before the remaining columns (see below). These optional columns are ignored by the function, but you may wish to use them for readability. For example, you might choose to include columns such as block number, trial number and condition. The argument colskip (see above) must be set to the number of optional columns plus one.

x1, x2, ... - activation of any number of input elements. There must be one column for each input element. Each row is one trial. In simple applications, one element is used for each stimulus (e.g. a simulation of blocking (Kamin, 1969), A+, AX+, would have two inputs, one for A and one for X). In simple applications, all present elements have an activation of 1 and all absence elements have an activation of 0. However, slpMack75 supports any real number for activations, e.g. one might use values between 0 and 1 to represent differing cue saliences.

t - Teaching signal (a.k.a. lambda). Traditionally, 1 is used to represent the presence of the outcome, and 0 is used to represent the absence of the outcome, although slpMack75 supports any real values for lambda. If you are planning to use multiple outcomes, see Note 2.

Argument xtdo (eXTenDed Output) - if set to TRUE, function will additionally return trial-level data including attentional strengths and the updated associative strengths after each trial (see Value).

Value

Returns a list containing three components (if xtdo = FALSE) or five components (if xtdo = TRUE, xoutw and xouta is also returned):

suma

Vector of summed associative strength for each trial.

w

Vector of final associative strengths.

alpha

Vector of final attentional weights.

xoutw

Matrix of trial-level data of the associative strengths at the end of the trial, after each has been updated.

xouta

Matrix of trial-level data of the attentional strengths at the end of the trial, after each has been updated.

Note

1. Mackintosh (1975) did not formalise how to update the cues' associability, but described when associability increases or decreases in Equation 4 and 5. He assumed that the change in alpha would reflect the difference between the prediction error generated by the current cue and the combined influence (a sum) of all other cues. Le Pelley et al. (2016) provided a linear function in Equation 2 that adheres to this description. This expression is probably the simplest way to express Mackintosh's somewhat vague description in mathematical terms. A linear function is also easier to computationally implement. So we decided to use Equation 2 from Le Pelley et al. (2016) for updating attentional strengths.

2. At present, only single-outcome experiments are officially supported. If you want to simulate a two-outcome study, consider using +1 for one outcome, and -1 for the other outcome. Alternatively, run a separate simulation for each outcome.

Author(s)

Lenard Dome, Andy Wills, Tom Beesley

References

Kamin, L.J. (1969). Predictability, surprise, attention and conditioning. In Campbell, B.A. & Church, R.M. (eds.), Punishment and Aversive Behaviour. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1969, pp.279-296.

Le Pelley, M. E., Mitchell, C. J., Beesley, T., George, D. N., & Wills, A. J. (2016). Attention and associative learning in humans: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 142(10), 1111–1140. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000064

Mackintosh, N.J. (1975). A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement, Psychological Review, 82, 276-298.

Wills, A.J., O'Connell, G., Edmunds, C.E.R., & Inkster, A.B.(2017). Progress in modeling through distributed collaboration: Concepts, tools, and category-learning examples. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 66, 79-115.


catlearn documentation built on April 4, 2023, 5:12 p.m.