dat.mccurdy2020: Studies on the Generation Effect

dat.mccurdy2020R Documentation

Studies on the Generation Effect

Description

Results from 126 articles that examined the so-called ‘generation effect’.

Usage

dat.mccurdy2020

Format

The data frame contains the following columns:

article numeric article identifier
experiment character experiment (within article) identifier
sample numeric sample (within experiment) identifier
id numeric row identifier
pairing numeric identifier to indicate paired conditions within experiments
yi numeric mean recall rate for the condition
vi numeric corresponding sampling variance
ni numeric number of participants for the condition
stimuli numeric number of stimuli for the condition
condition factor condition (‘read’ or ‘generate’)
gen_difficulty factor generation difficulty (‘low’ or ‘high’)
manip_type factor manipulation type of the generate versus read condition (using a ‘within’ or ‘between’ subjects design)
present_style factor presentation style (‘mixed’ or ‘pure’ list presentation)
word_status factor word status (‘words’, ‘non-words’, or ‘numbers’)
memory_test factor memory test (‘recognition’, ‘cued recall’, or ‘free recall’)
memory_type factor memory type (‘item’, ‘source’, ‘font color’, ‘font type’, ‘order’, ‘cue word’, ‘background color’, or ‘location’)
gen_constraint factor generation constraint (‘low’, ‘medium’, or ‘high’)
learning_type factor learning type (‘incidental’ or ‘intentional’)
stimuli_relation factor stimuli relation (‘semantic’, ‘category’, ‘antonym’, ‘synonym’, ‘rhyme’, ‘compound words’, ‘definitions’, or ‘unrelated’)
gen_mode factor generation mode (‘verbal/speaking’, ‘covert/thinking’, or ‘writing/typing’)
gen_task factor generation task (‘anagram’, ‘letter transposition’, ‘word fragment’, ‘sentence completion’, ‘word stem’, ‘calculation’, or ‘cue only’)
attention factor attention (‘divided’ or ‘full’)
pacing factor pacing (‘self-paced’ or ‘timed’)
filler_task factor filler task (‘yes’ or ‘no’)
age_grp factor age group (‘younger’ or ‘older’ adults)
retention_delay factor retention delay (‘immediate’, ‘short’, or ‘long’)

Details

The generation effect is the memory benefit for self-generated compared with read or experimenter-provided information (Jacoby, 1978; Slamecka & Graf, 1978). In a typical study, participants are presented with a list of stimuli (usually words or word pairs). For half of the stimuli, participants self-generate a target word (e.g., open–cl____), while for the other half, participants simply read an intact target word (e.g., above–below). On a later memory test for the target words, the common finding is that self-generated words are better remembered than read words (i.e., the generation effect).

Although several theories have been proposed to explain the generation effect, there is still some debate on the underlying memory mechanism(s) contributing to this phenomenon. The meta-analysis by McCurdy et al. (2020) translated various theories on the generation effect into hypotheses that could then be tested in moderator analyses based on a dataset containing 126 articles, 310 experiments, and 1653 mean recall estimates collected under various conditions.

Detailed explanations of the various variables coded (and how these can be used to test various hypotheses regarding the generation effect) can be found in the article. The most important variable is condition, which denotes whether a particular row of the dataset corresponds to the results of a ‘read’ or a ‘generate’ condition.

The data structure is quite complex. Articles may have reported the findings from multiple experiments involving one or multiple samples that were examined under various conditions. The pairing variable indicates which rows of the dataset represent a pairing of a read condition with one or multiple corresponding generate conditions within an experiment. A pairing may involve the same sample of subjects (when using a within-subjects design for comparing the conditions) or different samples (when using a between-subjects design).

Concepts

psychology, memory, proportions, raw means, multilevel models, cluster-robust inference

Author(s)

Wolfgang Viechtbauer, wvb@metafor-project.org, https://www.metafor-project.org

Source

McCurdy, M. P., Viechtbauer, W., Sklenar, A. M., Frankenstein, A. N., & Leshikar, E. D. (2020). Theories of the generation effect and the impact of generation constraint: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27(6), 1139–1165. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01762-3

References

Slamecka, N. J., & Graf, P. (1978). The generation effect: Delineation of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4(6), 592–604. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.4.6.592

Jacoby, L. L. (1978). On interpreting the effects of repetition: Solving a problem versus remembering a solution. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17(6), 649–668. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(78)90393-6

Examples

### copy data into 'dat' and examine data
dat <- dat.mccurdy2020
head(dat)

## Not run: 

### load metafor package
library(metafor)

### fit multilevel mixed-effects meta-regression model
res <- rma.mv(yi, vi, mods = ~ condition,
              random = list(~ 1 | article/experiment/sample/id, ~ 1 | pairing),
              data=dat, sparse=TRUE, digits=3)
res

### proportion of total amount of heterogeneity due to each component
data.frame(source=res$s.names, sigma2=round(res$sigma2, 3),
   prop=round(res$sigma2 / sum(res$sigma2), 2))

### apply cluster-robust inference
sav <- robust(res, cluster=article)
sav

### estimated average recall rate in read and generate conditions
predict(sav, newmods = c(0,1), digits=3)

### use methods from clubSandwich package
sav <- robust(res, cluster=article, clubSandwich=TRUE)
sav


## End(Not run)

metadat documentation built on April 6, 2022, 5:08 p.m.